If you lost your job
today, what would you be more likely to do?
A)
Use the
excess money from your last paycheck to splurge on dinner at your favorite
restaurant
or
B)
Increase
the contribution you make to your savings this month
According to the ‘life-history theory’, A
represents more of a fast life strategy, while B represents a slow life
strategy. Fast and slow life strategies explain how different groups of people
behave as a function of their evolutionary environment. Individuals who live in
a dangerous or stressful environment have to spend their resources on surviving
in the present moment, or have to have fast strategies for survival. For
example, females who live in an environment with a high rate of disease or
multiple threats to survival start having children at younger ages to increase
the likelihood of having at least one child reach adulthood. In contrast, individuals
who live in a safe and enriched environment spend their resources on
flourishing in the future, or have slow strategies for survival. For example,
females who live in an environment with an abundance of resources and
protection start having children at later ages to increase the amount of
resources that offspring has available to them, and thus have more advantages
throughout development. In the example above, both solutions serve a positive
purpose. Option A serves the purpose of mitigating stress and worry while
option B will result in more available resources for the challenging days to
come.
A group of
psychologists recently published a paper examining how fast and slow life
strategies relate to socioeconomic status. Previous research has shown that
when people are worried about money, some people increase their spending and
risk-taking while others conserve. In a study published this year, Dr.
Griskevicius and his colleagues at the University of Minnesota reported their
findings from three experiments which address the question:
Does how much money we had when we were children determine our financial decisions as adults?
To answer this
question they recruited three samples of adults and asked them a number of questions
about their financial stability as children. Then they proceeded to conduct
three experiments. In each of these studies, half of the participants were
primed with information about financial insecurity, the recession condition,
and half weren’t primed, the control condition. In the first study, they showed half the
participants images of recession related photos (e.g., photos of unemployment
lines and soup kitchens). The other half of the participants viewed images that
were neutral like trees and hills. After viewing the photos, all of the
participants completed computer tasks where they made decisions about financial
risk-taking and delay of gratification. For example, in the risk-taking
computer task, they were given the choice between having a 50% chance of
getting $10 or a 10% chance of getting $50. In the delay of gratification task,
the participants were given the choice of receiving $10 today or $20 in 2
weeks. What they found was that adults who had financial worries as children
were more likely to take financial risks and choose not to delay financial
rewards if they were in the recession condition.
To further test this
finding, they conducted a 2nd study. They gave a new set of participants
one of 2 fake newspaper articles to read. In the recession condition, the
participants read a “doom & gloom” newspaper article about the current
state of our economy and the negative implications it will have for young
adults today. In the control condition, individuals read a neutral article of
similar length. After reading the articles, the participants played a game where
they saw images of common luxury brands (e.g., Rolex, Porsche) and used a
joystick to “pull” the brands they like towards themselves and “push away” the
brands they didn’t. Consistent with the first experiment, they found that the
participants in the recession condition who grew up with concerns about
financial security pulled luxury brands faster and with more force. In
contrast, participants who reported financial security during childhood and
read the article about the recession were more likely to push the luxury brands
away.
Finally, they
conducted a 3rd experiment to see if stress had anything to do with these
findings. In this study, they had half the participants read the recession
article and the other half read the neutral article. Then they played a balloon
game, where they get 10 cents for every pump of air that goes into the balloon,
but they lose all of their money if the balloon pops. In this study they also
measured how much stress hormone each participant produced during the balloon
game. They found that participants in the recession condition with financial
insecurity during childhood produced more stress hormones and also took more
risks during the balloon game. Thus, they not only found that childhood financial
security determines your financial behavior as an adult, but also that a
biological stress system was motivating this behavior.
Together, these three
studies mean that if you were concerned about money and resources during
childhood, you are more likely to spend and make risky financial decisions than
when you are stressed about money. What’s most interesting about the findings
of these three experiments is that under the control conditions, when people
are not primed with financial concerns, the participants all perform similarly.
Many psychologists have written about the phenomena that, despite many programs
aimed at income inequality, people who were raised with few resources seldom
escape their financial problems. These findings are evidence that this
phenomenon may not be driven by daily habits, but rather differences in how
individuals behave in times of stress.
The take home message
that can clearly be gleaned from the findings of these studies is simple.
Regardless of your perceptions of financial security during childhood, we all
could benefit from a bit more self-monitoring when it comes to financial
decisions. The next time you decide to buy something or abstain from buying
something, think about whether your motivation to choose one over the other is
related to a recent financial stressor. If so, perhaps the timing is wrong.
No comments:
Post a Comment